Psychology 475/575
Cognitive Development
Dr. Lou Moses
Midterm 2 Answer Key
Note: The information presented below is not intended to represent complete model answers to the questions. Rather it is intended to give you at least a rough sense of what was necessary to do well on the exam.
1) Text pp. 302-304
The constraints approach to semantic development is designed to answer the
question of how children figure out the meanings of words, when those words can
have an indefinitely large number of possible meanings. According to the
constraints approach children bring to the word learning situation biases or
tendencies to favor some hypotheses about word meanings over others.
Examples of constraints are the whole object assumption ( assume that a new
word refers to the whole object rather than to a part or property), the
taxonomic assumption (assume that the word refers to a category of things of
like kind rather than to a thematic relation in which the thing participates),
and the mutual exclusivity assumption (assume that things can only have one
label; a new word must then refer to something for which the child does not
already have a label).
2) Lecture by Dr. Baldwin, reader (Baldwin article).
The ability to draw inferences about the intentions of others helps infants to determine the intended referents of speakers’ utterances (are they talking about X or Y?). Baldwin’s research on “discrepant labeling” suggests that appreciating others’ intentions does affect language learning. In this research 18-month-old infants were focused on a different toy than that which the experimenter was targeting with a novel label.. nevertheless, they did not link the novel label to their own toy. Instead, they appropriately checked to see what the experimenter was looking at and linked the label to that object. In other words, they appeared to be taking into account the experimenter’s intentions in making correct word-object links.
3) Lecture; Wynn article in the reader; text (pp. 129-132).
Recent research suggests that infants can both perceive numerical information and calculate the results of simple arithmetic operations on small numbers of items. Evidence for the former ability comes from experiments showing that infants can discriminate between small numbers of items (e.g., 2 vs. 3) on the basis of number. Evidence for the latter ability comes from the Wynn experiments showing that infants apparently represent the number of items present behind a screen and then make inferences about how that number changes as items are either added or subtracted.
4) Lecture; Mischel article in reader
Delay of gratification is the ability to postpone a smaller, immediate reward in order to obtain a larger delayed one (e.g., one cookie now vs. two later). Factors that affect children's ability to delay gratification include age, keeping the object out of sight; engaging in distracting activity; think about abstract vs. arousing properties of the reward.
5) Lecture.
Conceptual competence refers to having the basic concepts in a particular domain. Utilizational competence refers to knowing when to use these concepts. Research with Brazilian child street vendors suggests that these children have conceptual competence but not utilizational competence in the domain of numerical reasoning. They can do complex calculations when dealing with the familiar goods they sell everyday suggesting that they have the conceptual competence, but they cannot do the very same calculations when they are presented in an abstract context, suggesting that they do not have utilizational competence.
6) Lecture; text p 221
Self-recognition has been measured using the mirror task. Infants are first
surreptitiously marked on the nose with rouge. They are then placed in front of
a mirror. If infants then reach up and touch the mark on their nose (as opposed
to touching the mark reflected in the mirror), they are said to have recognized
themselves.
Self-recognition has also been measured by looking at whether infants recognize
when they are being imitated (and hence see themselves reflected in the actions
of another person). Infants are confronted with two experimenters, one of whom
is imitating them and the other of whom is imitating a different baby (or in
the 2nd experiment is merely performing actions that are in temporal synchrony
with those of the current baby). Recognition of imitation is reflected by
longer looking times with respect to the imitating experimenter.
The mirror task suggests that children begin to recognize themselves between
about 15 and 24 months of age. The imitation task suggests that they recognize
themselves as early as 14 months.
Self recognition does relate to other important developments. Examples include
embarrassment (children who self recognize are more likely to show
embarrassment in social situations such as being asked to dance by the
experimenter), empathy (children who self recognize are more likely to show
empathy, as in the Bischof-Kohler experiment in which they attempt to console
the experimenter and/or the bear) and the development of standards (an
awareness of success vs. failure at meeting standards emerges at about the same
time as self recognition).
7) Lecture, text (pp. 102-105), DeLoache article in reader.
DeLoache believes it is a dual coding problem (children have difficulty in thinking of the scale model as both an object in its own right as well as a symbol). Alternative explanations and evidence against them are as follows. 1. Memory. Maybe children have forgotten where little snoopy was hidden. Against this, they can go back to find little snoopy after unsuccessfully failing to find big snoopy. 2. Language. Maybe children are confused by the language, especially the phrase “the same place”. Against this children do well when a photograph rather than a scale model is used, even though the language is very similar. 3. Perceptual Similarity. Perhaps children simply don’t recognize the correspondence between the scale model and the larger room because they look too different. Against this, when the rooms are made to be identical, 2-year-olds still find it difficult to locate big snoopy. Evidence positively supporting DeLoache’s explanation comes from (a) model manipulation experiments (playing with the model causes performance to deteriorate; putting the model behind a glass barrier enhances performance), (b) photograph experiments (children understand the relation between a photo and the larger space better than that between a scale model and the larger space) (c) comparable findings from pretense (2 1/2 -year-olds have difficulty pretending that an object with a known function could be something entirely different; they have less difficulty, however, pretending that an object with ambiguous or multiple functions could be something different) and (d) the Shrunken Room experiment (children have little difficulty when dual coding is not required. i.e., when the room is a shrunken version of the original rather than a representation of it).